The value of feedback for improving second-language (L2)
writing skills has been widely established in L2 writing literature
(Bitchener & Ferris, 2012; Ferris, 2012). For example, providing
feedback on content, structure, and style all seen to have immediate
and long-lasting effects on L2 learners’ writing abilities and
motivation. Still, many writing instructors and researchers have argued
that the need for providing corrective feedback (CF) on grammar
outweighs the lack of empirical research supporting its practice
(Truscott, 1996). It was with this in mind that Truscott (2007)
summarized the empirical findings of CF on L2 grammar and again found it
to be ineffective in the short term. Truscott also determined that a
more disciplined approach to grammar instruction was necessary to
promote efficient language learning and acquisition.
Grammar error correction has been debated since early
scholarship on L2 acquisition (Corder, 1967; James, 1998). Responses to
this debate often focused on how CF might improve subsequent grammar
usage and whether it would have a lasting impact on learners’
interlanguage (Ferris, 1999). Ferris argued for the use of CF, but also
encouraged ongoing research, noting that “we must take Truscott’s claims
and challenge seriously…helping students to develop their written
language skills and improve their accuracy in writing is too important
to be ruled on hastily” (p. 10).
The effects of instruction on learners’ errors has since been
determined to only be noticable longitudinally (Ellis, 1990). Therefore,
to establish the effects of CF on grammar acquisition, longitudinal
studies of many years in length (Jackson, 2013) are required to observe
changes in learners’ grammar acquisition. Nevertheless, very few CF
research studies have used a longitudinal design (see Sasaki, 2009), and
it has been difficult to establish whether CF can have a lasting impact
on L2 grammar acquisition. Much scholarship continues to examine its
short-term influence on learners’ grammatical development (Truscott,
2007). Due to the application of short-term cross-sectional
interlanguage development studies, it is highly unlikely that the CF
provided could realistically provide insight on its long-term impact.
With these established limitations of CF treatments on L2
writing, it appears that CF research has little to offer second language
acquisition (SLA) researchers and classroom practitioners. However,
addressing errors in writing is vital to the overall comprehension of
current and possibly future compositions of a learner; thus, applying
SLA theory to CF research and practice remains an important goal to
pursue.
Application of SLA Theory to the Study of CF
Transfer and Variability
Traditionally, past CF studies have not drawn from SLA theory
to substantiate their hyphotheses that the application of CF on errors
would improve the accuracy of grammatical features in subsequent L2
compositions. Nevertheless, SLA theory is important, as it provides the
foundation for explaining the results of many previous CF studies. From
an SLA perspective (e.g., Towell & Hawkins, 1994), factors
investigated have included:
- first-language (L1) transfer;
-
deficits in L2 learning;
-
learning variability among learners;
-
staged development (e.g., went – goed – went), as seen in Figure 1; and
-
language systematicity as in the case of morpheme acquisition orders (see Table 1).
In most CF studies, (1) L1 transfer, (2) deficits in L2
learning as represented by errors or avoidance of grammatical features,
and (3) learning variability are evident when researchers report their
findings in the results and analysis sections. These factors complicate
coding and statistical measurements as error types, and the frequency of
those errors varies across learners and writing tasks. In fact, task
factors (e.g., choice of grammar, task type, time-on-task, cognitive
load, task repetition, etc.) influence language usage to such a degree
that it is difficult to compare a partipant’s pretest and posttest
errors, making the results of a study especially difficult to interpret
when a large number of participants are involved. To resolve this
methodological limitation and improve the validity and reliability of
these studies, research that uses grammar tests that target specific
items of interest and simulate actual writing task conditions are
needed. In the meantime, much can be gained from case studies, as they
provide valuable insight into CF and grammatical acquisition (Ferris,
Hsiang, Sinha, & Senna, 2013).
Staged Development
SLA theory can be more helpful in describing the changes in
interlanguage through staged development (see Figure 1). Here, learners
go through three stages of learning as depicted by a U-shaped learning
curve for irregular verbs. In Stage 1, after learners received
instruction, they produce correct forms; however, as time goes on,
deviant forms (errors) emerge due to backsliding (Stage 2). In the final
stage, as learners become more proficent with the grammatical item,
near perfect grammar usage is observed. Therefore, great care should be
taken when describing the resulting changes in grammatical accuracy
rates, as the determination of which stage learners are in affects
whether CF was successful in aiding interlanguage development.
Figure 1. Staged development of irregular verbs

Adapted from Oshita (2000) and O'Grady (2005)
Systematicity
Another point that is rarely mentioned in CF studies is the
role of systematicity in the acquisition of grammatical features. In
Table 1, morpheme acquisition orders for L1 Spanish and L1 Japanese
learners of English indicate that the success of CF may depend on
learners’ L1, as well as the grammatical feature that is being
corrected. At this point in time, further research is needed to
establish the orders of acquisition for a variety of grammatical items,
and align these orders with the application of CF to increase the
likelihood that CF treatment could enhance grammar
acquisition.
Table 1. Morpheme Acquisition Orders for Spoken English
Natural Order
(Dulay & Burt, 1973) |
NICT JLE Corpus
(Izumi & Isahara, 2004) |
1 plural –s “Books” |
1 possessive –’s |
2 progressive –ing “John going” |
2 progressive –ing |
3 copula be “John is here” |
3 copula be |
4 auxiliary be “John is going” |
4 third-person singular |
5 articles (the/a) “The books” |
5 plural –s |
6 irregular past tense “John went” |
6 auxiliary be |
7 third person –s “John likes books” |
7 irregular past tense (went) |
8 possessive –’s “John’s book” |
8 articles (a/the) |
In summary, the application of SLA theories can help provide an
estimation of when CF is likely to influence grammar learning and
acquisition, and learners’ interlanguage, and the type and saliency of
CF may be a useful indicator of its successful application.
Application of CF in Accordance With Interlanguage Stage
The determination of the CF type and saliency of CF is likely
to be dependent on a learner’s interlanguage stage for any particular
grammatical item. Corrective feedback type is divided into focused and
unfocused feedback. Focused CF targets a limited number of items,
whereas unfocused CF covers more grammatical features and is more
comprehensive. Corrective feedback saliency refers to the degree to
which explicit grammar knowledge is provided to the learner. Direct CF
shows learners where the error is and replaces the mistake or error with
the correct form. Metalinguistic CF gives an explanation of the correct
grammatical form. While indirect CF may or may not show that an error
or mistake was made, no correct form is made available to the learner
(for a thorough description of CF typology, see Ellis, 2009).
Table 2. Interlanguage Stage and Applicable Corrective Feedback
|
CF Type and CF Saliency (with error/mistake indicated) |
Interlanguage
Stage (Accuracy) |
CF Type |
CF Saliency |
Focused |
Unfocused |
Direct |
Metalinguistic |
Indirect |
1 (0–25%) |
Error |
- |
Error |
Error |
- |
1–2 (25–50%) |
Error |
- |
Error |
Error |
- |
2–3 (50–75%) |
Mistake/Error |
Mistake/Error |
Mistake/Error |
Mistake/Error |
- |
3 (75–100%) |
Mistake |
Mistake |
Mistake |
Mistake |
Mistake |
In Table 2, the interlanguage stage and CF type and CF saliency
are correlated. In the early stages of grammar learning, it is more
likely that the learner will make errors due to lack of grammatical
knowledge. On the other hand, at Interlanguage Stages 2 and 3, the
learner has more control over the grammatical item, but still can make
either an error or a mistake. The differences in
whether an error or mistake is made can inform how instructors provide
CF. For instance, when a learner does not have sufficient knowledge to
self-correct an error, more direct and focused CF should be provided to
successfully remediate the error. On the other hand, mistakes could be
treated with less CF intervention, allowing for the learner to access
his or her grammar knowledge. The reason for these different CF
treatments is to promote language development, as well as eventual
learner autonomy as learners’ proficiency improves over time. To improve
CF practice, it is clear that ongoing research into the effects of CF
on grammar development is urgently needed.
Goals of CF Research and Classroom Practice
While corrective feedback research focuses on examining several
issues, CF and SLA research are both committed to the improvement of L2
pedagogy. In the long term, it is presumed that SLA research will lead
to an empirically supported strategic CF approach that will lead to
effective language learning. Current research on the effects of CF
treatment on learners’ errors may not necessarily result in observable
changes in accuracy in the short term; however, the presence of CF alone
may encourage self-monitoring of language usage for self-correction
(Cresswell, 2000). For instance, CF may trigger a response which signals
to the learner that their current language should be altered, which may
even prevent fossilized language (Han, 2013), and promote grammar
acquisition.
With the application of CF on L2 learners’ written production,
it is often assumed that accuracy of grammatical forms will immediately
improve and be maintained. It is likely that when CF is successful, as
in the case where mistakes are self-corrected (Suzuki, 2008), the uptake
of the correct form can be maintained for long periods of time.
Mistakes, as opposed to errors, are grammatical features that have been
successfully learned, but are occasionally produced incorrectly. Errors,
on the other hand, are less likely to be within a learner’s ability to
readily correct due to a lack of accessible grammatical knowledge under
writing task conditions. When the application of CF is ineffective, this
indicates that additional instruction and practice of that item may be
required. It also shows the current interlanguage state of the learner
and the next steps to take to improve the learner’s grammar development.
It can be argued that when CF is successful in facilitating the
correction of errors and acquisition of those forms, preemptive
classroom instruction (Ellis, Basturkmen, & Loewen, 2001) may
facilitate grammar accuracy as part of classroom instruction and pretask
planning (Foster & Skehan, 1996).
Preemptive Grammar Instruction
It was through the efforts of many researchers (e.g., Ellis,
Sheen, Murakami, & Takashima, 2008; Bitchener & Knoch,
2010a, 2010b; Sheen, 2010) that CF research produced findings that could
enhance pedagogical practices in the classroom. If CF can help make a
group of language learners monitor their language and shift toward
better language usage (Schmidt, 1990), it is likely that improvement in
whole-class instruction, rather than individualized CF, would be a more
effective pedagogical approach. Through continued persistence in
researching the effects of CF, it was revealed that some grammatical
items (e.g., articles) are ready to be processed through the application
of CF (see Piennemann & Kessler, 2011). In other words, when
the application of CF is effective in developing interlanguage,
improvements in instructional task design (Bygate, Skehan, &
Swain, 2001) may help to preempt potential errors, rather than
correcting by relying on posttask remediation in the form of CF. Thus,
the promotion of preemptive instruction for grammatical features that
were maintained after CF is seen as a step forward in improving the
accuracy rates of targeted items (Ellis et al., 2001).
Summary
The role of CF on L2 writing and L2 grammar development is
highly complex. L2 acquisition theory and CF application to learners’
mistakes and errors still requires more integration to enhance research
practices for examining how CF can be used to facilitate the advancement
of learners’ interlanguage. Many CF studies to date have only
marginally supported investigations with the knowledge obtained through
empirical SLA research. Continuing CF research that utilizes sound
methodological practices derived from SLA findings, as argued in this
article, may be another step in understanding how CF can be used to
support learners’ L2 grammatical development in L2 writing.
References
Bitchener, J., & Ferris, D. R. (2012). Written
corrective feedback in second language acquisition and
writing. New York, NY: Routledge.
Bitchener, J., & Knoch, U. (2010a). Raising the
linguistic accuracy level of advanced L2 writers with written corrective
feedback. Journal of Second Language Writing, 19,
207–217.
Bitchener, J., & Knoch, U. (2010b). The contribution of
written corrective feedback to language development: A ten month
investigation. Applied Linguistics, 31(2),
193–121.
Bygate, M., Skehan, P., & Swain, M. (Eds.). (2001). Researching pedagogic tasks: Second language learning, teaching
and testing. Harlow, United Kingdom: Pearson
Education.
Corder, S. P. (1967). The significance of learners’ errors. International Review of Applied Linguistics 5,
161–170.
Cresswell, A. (2000). Self-monitoring in student writing. ELT Journal, 54, 235–244.
Dulay, H. C., & Burt, M. K. (1973). Should we teach
children syntax? Language Learning, 23, 245–258.
Ellis, R. (1990). Instructed second language
acquisition. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Ellis, R. (2009). A typology of written corrective feedback types. ELT J, 63, 97–107.
Ellis, R., Basturkmen, H., & Loewen, S. (2001).
Preemptive focus on form in the ESL classroom. TESOL Quarterly,
35, 407–432.
Ellis, R., Sheen, Y., Murakami, M., & Takashima, H.
(2008). The effects of focused and unfocused written corrective feedback
in an English as a foreign language context. System,
36, 353–371.
Ferris, D. R. (1999). The case of grammar correction in L2
writing classes: A response to Truscott (1996). Journal of
Second Language Writing, 8, 1–11.
Ferris, D. R. (2012). Written corrective feedback in second
language acquisition and writing studies. Language Teaching,
45, 446–459.
Ferris, D. R., Hsiang, L., Sinha, A., & Senna, M.
(2013). Written corrective feedback for individual L2 writers. Journal of Second Language Writing, 22,
307–329.
Foster, P., & Skehan, P. (1996). The influence of
planning on performance in task-based learning. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 18(3), 299–324.
Han, Z. (2013). Forty years later: Updating the fossilization
hypothesis. Language Teaching, 46, 133–171.
Izumi, E., & Isahara, H. (2004). Investigation into
language learners’ acquisition order based on an error analysis of a
learner corpus. IWLeL 2004: An Interactive
Workshop on Language e-Learning, 63–71. Retrieved from
http://dspace.wul.waseda.ac.jp/dspace/bitstream/2065/1396/1/07.pdf
Jackson, D. O. (2013). Longitudinal research. In P. Robinson
(Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of second language acquisition (pp. 404–405). New York, NY:
Routledge.
James, C. (1998). Errors in language learning and use:
Exploring error analysis. London, England: Longman.
O’Grady, W. (2005). How children learn
language. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University
Press.
Oshita, H. (2000). What is happened may not be what appears to
be happening: A corpus study of passive unaccusatives in L2 English. Second Language Research, 16, 293–324.
Pienemann, M., & Kessler, J.-U. (Eds.). (2011). Studying processability theory. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Sasaki, M. (2009). Changes in English as a foreign language
students’ writing over 3.5 years: A sociocognitive account. In R. M.
Manchón (Ed.), Writing in foreign language contexts: Learning, teaching, and research (pp. 49–76).
Bristol, England:Multilingual Matters.
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second
language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129–158.
Sheen, Y. (2010). Differential effects of oral and written
corrective feedback in the ESL classroom. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 32, 203–234.
Suzuki, M. (2008). Japanese learners’ self revisions and peer
revisions of their written compositions in English. TESOL
Quarterly, 42, 209–233.
Towell, R., & Hawkins, R. (1994). Approaches
to second language acquisition. Clevedon, England:
Multilingual Matters.
Truscott, J. (1996). The case against grammar correction in L2
writing classes. Language Learning, 46, 327–369.
Truscott, J. (2004). Dialogue: Evidence and conjecture on the
effects of correction: A response to Chandler. Journal of
Second Language Writing, 13, 337–343.
Truscott, J. (2007). The effect of error correction on
learners’ ability to write accurately. Journal of Second
Language Writing, 16, 255–272.
Robert Taferner has been teaching English in Japan
since 1993. He holds an MAT-TESOL from the School for International
Training in Vermont, USA. His research interests include pragmatics,
second language acquisition, and materials development. |